Evidence Based Education
A Meta-Analysis and Literature Review of Language Programs
Article Key Findings:
For the purposes of this article, I conducted a met-analysis of 63 language program studies. Despite the fact that this meta-analysis was conducted 20 years after the National Reading Panel meta-analysis, with studies included up to 2022, I found the near identical mean effect size for phonics, of .45. I also conducted a secondary meta-analysis of 13 phonics meta-analyses conducted over the last 25 years, which found a mean effect size of phonics, of .43. This helps to show that despite the fact that the NRP meta-analysis was conducted 20 years ago, its findings are still valid today. Moreover, not only did I find the same mean effect size, but I found the same general trends about phonics education held true as well.
I also found that phonics focused programs consistently outperformed Balanced Literacy programs across all demographics. Indeed, phonics on average almost doubled the effects found for Balanced Literacy for grades 1-2, for class based instruction, and for at risk learners.
Phonics interventions showed efficacious results, both for early primary instruction, and for older students with reading deficits. This suggests that students should receive phonics instruction during their foundational education years and that if they miss this instruction that they benefit from getting it later on.
Purpose of Analysis:
I often see the claim that a program is the best program or the most evidence-based program. However, these types of statements are rarely followed up by sufficient evidence. To prove something is the best program, I would ideally like to see multiple meta-analyses showing that a program has greater evidence of efficacy than other programs studied. Of course, even with this type of evidence, said analysis would only show that the program is better than the other programs studied, according to the currently available evidence most language programs have little to no research done, let alone a meta-analysis. To make matters more complex, results are context dependent, the program that works best for one grade or one outcome, might be completely different from what works best for a different grade/outcome. The reality is that proving a certain program is the best, is likely an intellectually fruitless endeavor.
However, I do believe we make educated guesses about which programs work better by conducting meta-analyses of the available evidence. While research studies do not always exactly match the conditions of the real world, by conducting meta-analyses of individual programs, we can gain a more objective proxy of what we have evidence for working. This is not to say that programs that show low results in meta-analyses are inherently bad or that programs which show high results are inherently good, but rather we can make our most educated guess as to what works, by using this method.
To this end, I have been conducting a series of small non-peer reviewed meta-analyses on my website on popular language programs. Ideally this information would be best conducted in the form of a published peer-reviewed meta-analysis. However, most programs do not have a meta-analysis conducted on them and some of the meta-analyses conducted appear to have limited scopes. This research has been my attempt to help make the available scientific literature on the topic more useful and meaningful to teachers; however, I recognize that it has severe limitations, in that it is not published peer-reviewed work, and I do not have post-graduate qualifications.
Methodology:
For the purposes of this study, searches of studies were conducted on Education Source, Sage Pub, Google, and company websites, for the most popular language programs. Programs were selected based, on polling by teachers interested in the science of reading. All studies found with a control group, sufficient reporting data (meaning either effect sizes or sufficient data for me to calculate a proper effect size), and total samples above 10 were included in this analysis. No time restrictions were place on this inclusion criteria. In total 63 studies were included. Three studies were initially included, but later excluded (after review) due to non-equivalent pre-test scores. In all cases, effect sizes based on the post test results were extremely misleading. One study was by Ring, Et, al on Take Flight. The second study was by Stuart, Et, al, on Jolly Phonics. In the cases of the studies on Take Flight and Jolly Phonics, the Cohen's D formula provided an effect size that was exaggerated in comparison to the actual gains made. For the Take Flight study, the gains were statistically insignificant. For the Jolly Phonics study the treatment group did worse than the control group in terms of gains, but still ended up with a positive effect of .73, using Cohen's d. The third study excluded was on Remediation Plus, by Corcoran, Et al. The gains found by the treatment group, were very significant with an effect of over .50, however, the non equivalent groups, resulted in a much smaller Cohen's d effect sizes.
Effect sizes for studies with sample sizes above 50 per group were calculated with a Cohen’s d calculation. Effect sizes for studies with sample sizes below 50 were calculated with Hedge’s g. When effect sizes were already calculated by the peer reviewed author, these effect sizes were usually accepted as is, unless there was probable cause for an improperly calculated effect size, in which case, the effect size was double checked. If the effect sizes did not match, I used my own calculation. All effect size calculations were independently duplicated by a second analyst, to guarantee the reliability of the results, in the case of a disagreement by the second analyst, both persons re-did their calculations, and discussed to reach a consensus. Effect sizes, were not rounded, they were cut off at the hundredth place value. When the effect size calculation found the control group to outperform the treatment group, the effect size was entered into the meta-analysis as a negative effect size. Meta-analyses typically report on results with effect sizes. These effect sizes are meant to be interpreted in the following way:
However, I think this can be a reductive lense. As effect sizes tend to be context specific. For example, studies on reading tend to show lower results, whereas studies on math tend to show higher results. For this reason, I think effect sizes for single outcomes, programs, or pedagogical ideas are not helpful for teachers. I think it is more useful to compare the effect sizes of similar factors, so that teachers can see which factors have a stronger research base. For this purpose, I have compiled my research in language programs so far into the following infographics:
Results:
As can be seen in the above results. Phonics heavy programs outperformed Balanced Literacy programs across all grades, and sample types. Indeed phonics programs showed roughly double the impact for grades 1-2, struggling readers, and for class-based instruction. This research does not show support for the use of Balanced Literacy programs, over phonics focused programs, in any context.
Of course, Balanced Literacy is not the only other sub-type of phonics programming. Within this meta-analysis, I also had samples of Orton Gillingham programs, analytic programs, and synthetic programs. To compare the efficacy of these styles of phonics programs, I also found the mean effect size for each sub-type of phonics program. The results of which can be found below.
One interesting claim, I often see of effect size and meta-analysis research is that the study design biases the result. Indeed people often claim that RCT studies show substantially lower results. Similarly, study authors often claim the reason they found low results, was due to low teacher fidelity. With this in mind, I broke down my meta-analysis data across design variables, the results of which can be seen below.
Similar to the NRP meta-analysis, this meta-analysis found higher results for studies that did not track fidelity, than studies that did. This seriously calls into question the notion that fidelity should be the primary examined culprit, when study results are low. Indeed program type variables showed far more significant impacts on results. While RCT studies did show on average lower results than quasi-experimental studies, this difference was not statistically significant. This calls into question the idea that we should expect lower results from RCT studies.
The data showed higher outcomes for shorter phonics studies, and for shorter balanced literacy studies. Balanced literacy studies were in particular effected by this phenomenon, as many of the longest duration balanced literacy studies showed negative results.
Ultimately, this meta-analysis was not a peer-reviewed published study, which limits its credibility. However, its results are very comparable with the rest of the scientific literature. To help rectify this problem, I have included a literature review of all other meta-analyses I could find on this topic, starting with the NRP meta-analysis.
NRP Discussion:
Overall, we see three really clear patterns, within this meta-analysis. Firstly, phonics is most useful for younger students and becomes less useful over time. Indeed, we see a statistically insignificant effect for teaching phonics to students in grade 2 in up, overall, within this meta-analysis. I actually had the opportunity to discuss these specific results with Timothy Shanahan, who was one of the project leads. To paraphrase, he told me that we saw an inverse relationship, with older students getting lower results from phonics and younger students getting higher results. Now that being said, the grade 2 and grade 6 results are all lumped together, but from my discussions with Dr. Shanahan, I think we can assume that the grade 2 results are much higher than the grade 6 results.
Interestingly, we see here that the NRP evidence that phonics helps at-risk readers above grade 2 or dyslexic students, is quite weak, something Dr. Shanahan also conceded when he discussed the topic with me. Indeed opponents of phonics have used this information to try and discredit the use of phonics for remedial students. However, I would caution against this interpretation. What this is likely actually showing is that Dyslexic students learn to read more slowly and therefore require more instructional time spent on foundational knowledge. I also think the results might have been deflated at the time, by the studies chosen for this sample.
I also think it should be noted that the NRP meta-analysis found the same trends for both phonics overall and programs as I did. Both the NRP meta-analysis and mine meta-analysis found a mean effect size for phonics of .45. The NRP found Jolly Phonics to have the highest results for any individual program, it found synthetic phonics programs tended to have the higher results compared to analytic phonics. Moreover, it found on average lower results for Orton Gillingham programs. This is not to say that Orton Gillingham programs are bad programs, indeed, it is possible that these programs are helping students, in a way that the research is not capturing. However, meta-analysis data does seem to consistently find lower results for these programs, with the notable exception of the SPIRE program. Indeed, my meta-analysis result for Orton GIllingham programs, might be inflated due to my heavy weight for SPIRE, which has the highest results for this type of program and indeed some of the highest results overall.
Other Phonics Meta-Analyses:
In total I was able to find 15 meta-analyses including my own that looked at the efficacy of phonics. The average result of these meta-analyses was .54. However, the 2014 Garcia meta-analysis of the topic is clearly an extreme outlier. Indeed, I have no idea how they found this effect size, as I have never found a single phonics study with an effect size over 1.5, let alone find a mean average for over 2. If we correct for this outlier data, we get a mean effect size of .43, which I must point out, is with .02 of my meta-analysis of the subject. However, three of these studies (mine included) were not peer reviewed. If we remove these 3 studies and the outlier, we get a mean effect size of .57, which I must admit, I think is likely a bit high. However that being said, whereas I used to be thoroughly convinced that this was the best method for assessing the efficacy of phonics, I am now more skeptical, after completing this analysis. Personally, I would be more willing to accept the results of my own analysis or the NRP, over the results of a secondary meta-analysis, due to the amount of outlier data.
The results of phonics studies seem very context dependent on two factors: the age of the students and the program being used, whereas fidelity, experiment design, and duration seemed to have less impact on the results of phonics studies. Indeed, prior to starting this project, I was firmly convinced that the principles of phonics are more crucial to instruction outcomes than the program used. However, I am less confident in this belief now. As some phonics programs showed results that were below .20 and others showed results that were above .80. I think this fact is substantially damaging to the idea that as long as our program is based on the principles of science of reading that it is essentially evidence-based.
Whole Language Meta-Analyses:
As you can see from these charts there does seem to be strong evidence that Phonics has a stronger evidence base than Balanced Literacy, whereas Balanced Literacy has stronger evidence than Whole Language.
Orton Gillingham:
Orton Gillingham programs are very popular in the Science of Reading community and so I thought I would share the available meta-analysis data I had on the subject as well. However, the evidence on the topic, might be weaker than some might assume. Indeed, my meta-analysis results, which were low, appear to be by far the highest.
To help summarize all of this data, I have created a secondary meta-analysis, which shows the mean meta-analysis result for each program type, where I had available data.
As can be seen here, there appears to be strong evidence that a phonics program is better than a balanced literacy program and a Whole Language program.
Limitations:
This meta-analysis did not include a review of the number of studies excluded, within the systematic search.
Effect sizes were based on basic means and were not weighted according to sample size. Had studies been weighted by sample size, the mean effect for Balanced Literacy would be substantially lower.
Written by Nathaniel Hansford and Joshua King
Last Edited 2024-12-03 ***This article was updated to remove program rankings. As we no longer stand by the practice of directly ranking programs against each other, by effect size, alone. However, it should be noted that this research is now out of date, with our most current research and will be updated soon.
*Updates:
-Updated moderator variable data, based on a revised variable criterion
-Updated to add confidence intervals and regression analysis
-Updated to remove a Jolly Phonics study that was no longer publicly available
-Updated to include additional Reading Recovery studies listed on the Reading Recovery website
-Updated discussion write up
-Updated to remove studies that were non-equivalent at baseline
-Updated calculation methodology for better standardization to only include Cohen's d and Hedge's g.
References:
Other Meta-Analyses:
J, Hattie. (2022). Phonics. Visible Learning Metax. Retrieved from <https://www.visiblelearningmetax.com/influences/view/phonics_instruction>.
J, Hattie. (2022). Whole Language. Visible Learning Metax. Retrieved from <https://www.visiblelearningmetax.com/influences/view/whole_language_approach>.
NRP. (2001). Teaching Children to Read: An Evidence Based Assessment of the Scientific Literature on Reading Instruction. United States Government. Retrieved from <https://www.nichd.nih.gov/sites/default/files/publications/pubs/nrp/Documents/report.pdf>.
S, Graham, et al. (2017). Effectiveness of Literacy Programs Balancing Reading and Writing Instruction: A Meta-Analysis. Reading Research Quarterly. Volume 53: Issue 3.
Stevens, E. A., Austin, C., Moore, C., Scammacca, N., Boucher, A. N., & Vaughn, S. (2021). Current State of the Evidence: Examining the Effects of Orton-Gillingham Reading Interventions for Students With or at Risk for Word-Level Reading Disabilities. Exceptional Children, 87(4), 397–417. https://doi.org/10.1177/0014402921993406
LLI:
Center for Research Policy in Education. (2012). Efficacy of the Leveled Literacy Intervention System for K–2 Urban Students: An Empirical Evaluation of LLI in Denver Public Schools Study Dates: 2011–2012. University of Memphis. Retrieved from <https://www.fountasandpinnell.com/shared/resources/FP_LLI_Research_CREP-LLI-Efficacy-Full-Report-2012.pdf>.
Center for Research Policy in Education. (2010). Implementation of Effective Intervention: An Empirical Study to Evaluate the Efficacy of Fountas & Pinnell’s Leveled Literacy Intervention System (LLI) Retrieved from <https://www.fountasandpinnell.com/shared/resources/FP_LLI_Research_CREP-LLI-Efficacy-Full-Report-2010.pdf>.
Center for Research Policy in Education. (2016). The Efficacy of the Leveled Literacy Intervention System for Students in Grades 3–5: Data Summary Report for Abilene Independent School District 2015–2016. University of Memphis. Retrieved from <https://www.fountasandpinnell.com/shared/resources/LLI%203-5%202015_16-AISD_FULL_REPORT-web.pdf>.
Center for Research Policy in Education. (2015). The Efficacy of the Leveled Literacy Intervention System for Students in Grades 3–5: Data Summary Report for Denver Public Schools 2015–2016. University of Memphis. Retrieved from <https://www.fountasandpinnell.com/shared/resources/LLI%203-5%202015_16-DPS_FULL_REPORT-web.pdf>.
Center for Research Policy in Education. (2016). The Efficacy of the Leveled Literacy
Intervention System for Students in Grades 3–5: Data Summary Report for Sandwich Public Schools 2015–2016. University of Memphis. Retrieved from <https://www.fountasandpinnell.com/shared/resources/LLI%203-5%202015_16-SPS_FULL_REPORT-web.pdf>.
Heinmann. (2010). Levelled Literacy Intervention, Research and Data Collection Project. Fountas and Pinnell. Retrieved from <https://www.fountasandpinnell.com/shared/resources/FP_LLI_Research_Research-and-Data-Collection-Project-Report.pdf>.
Gonzalez, et al. (2018). Challenges in Adolescent Reading Intervention: Evidence from a Randomized Control Trial. Mathmatica Policy Research. Retrieved from <https://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/ED587404.pdf>.
Lang, Laura & Torgesen, Joseph & Vogel, William & Chanter, Carol & Lefsky, Evan & Petscher, Yaacov. (2009). Exploring the Relative Effectiveness of Reading Interventions for High School Students. Journal of Research on Educational Effectiveness. 2. 149-175. 10.1080/19345740802641535.
Metz, Tracie Jean, "A case study: Effects of using leveled literacy intervention on fourth and fifth grade students' reading achievement" (2014). ETD Collection for Fayetteville State University. AAI3581423.
https://digitalcommons.uncfsu.edu/dissertations/AAI3581423
J, Majewski. (2018). The effects of a leveled literacy inter acy intervention (LLI) on elementary age students reading below grade level. Rowan University. Retrieved from <https://rdw.rowan.edu/cgi/viewcontent.cgi?article=3574&context=etd>.
J, Hattie. (2021). Visible Learning Metax. Retrieved from <www.visiblelearningmetax.com>.
N, Hansford. (2021). Morphology. Pedagogy Non Grata. Retrieved from <https://nathanielhansford.wixsite.com/website/morphology>.
Linnea, et al. (2001). Systematic Phonics Instruction Helps Students Learn to Read: Evidence From the National Reading Panel’s Meta-Analysis. Ontario Institute for Studies in Education. Retrieved from <https://www.dyslexie.lu/JDI_02_02_04.pdf>.
Reading Recovery:
R, Colvin. Reading Recovery Revisited. The School Superintendent Association. Retrieved from <https://aasa.org/SchoolAdministratorArticle.aspx?id=15712> .
D’Agostino, J. V., & Harmey, S. J. (2016). An International Meta-Analysis of Reading Recovery. Journal of Education for Students Placed at Risk (JESPAR), 21(1), 29–46. https://doi.org/10.1080/10824669.2015.1112746
Gardner, J., Sutherland, A., & Meenan-Strain, C. (1998) Reading Recovery in Northern Ireland: The first two years. Belfast, Ireland: Blackstaff.
Schwartz, Robert. (2005). Literacy Learning of At-Risk First-Grade Students in the Reading Recovery Early Intervention.. Journal of Educational Psychology. 97. 257-267. 10.1037/0022-0663.97.2.257.
Sirinides, P., Gray, A., & May, H. (2018). The Impacts of Reading Recovery at Scale: Results From the 4-Year i3 External Evaluation. Educational Evaluation and Policy Analysis, 40(3), 316–335. https://doi.org/10.3102/0162373718764828
Burroughs-Lange, S. (2008). Comparison of literacy progress of young children in London Schools: A RR Follow-Up Study.London, UK: Institute of Education. Retrieved from https://www.ioe.ac.uk/Comparison_of_Literacy_Progress_of_Young_Children_in_London_Schools_-_A_Reading_Recovery_Follow_up_Study_.pdf
Hurry, J., & Sylva, K. (2007). Long-term outcomes of early reading intervention. Journal of Research in Reading, 30(3), 227–248. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9817.2007.00338.x
Pinnell, Html & Lyons, Carol & Deford, Diane & Bryk, Anthony & Seltzer, Michael. (1994). Comparing Instructional Models for the Literacy Education of High-Risk First Graders.
Reading Research Quarterly. 29. 10.2307/747736.
Holliman, A.J., and Hurry, J. (2013) The effects of Reading Recovery on children's literacy progress and Special Educational Needs status: A three-year follow-up study. Educational Psychology, 33(6), pp. 719-733
Shanahan, T., & Barr, R. (1995). Reading Recovery: an independent evaluation of the effects of an early instructional intervention for at-risk learners. Reading Research Quarterly, 30, 958–996. https://doi-org.ezproxy.lakeheadu.ca/10.2307/748206
Lyons, C. A. (1988). Reading Recovery: Early intervention for at-risk first graders (Educational Research Service Monograph). Arlington, VA: Educational Research Service. (ERIC Document Reproduction
Service No. ED303790).
DeFord, D., Pinnell, G. S., Lyons, C. A., & Young, P. (1987). Reading Recovery program: Report of the follow-up studies
(Vol. VII). Columbus, OH: The Ohio State University.
Empower Reading:
Lovett, M. W., Frijters, J. C., Wolf, M., Steinbach, K. A., Sevcik, R. A., & Morris, R. D. (2017). Early intervention for children at risk for reading disabilities: The impact of grade at intervention and individual differences on intervention outcomes. Journal of Educational Psychology, 109(7), 889-914. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/edu0000181
Jolly Phonics:
Nasrawi, A., & Al-Jamal, D. (2017). The Effect of Using Jolly Phonics on Jordanian First Grade Pupils’ Reading. International Online Journal of Education & Teaching, 4(2), 106–119.
Callinan, C., & van der Zee, E. (2010). A comparative study of two methods of synthetic phonics instruction for learning how to read: Jolly Phonics and THRASS. Psychology of Education Review, 34(1), 21–31.
M, Stuart. (1999). Getting ready for reading: Early phoneme awareness and phonics teaching improves reading and spelling in inner-city second language learners. British Journal of Psychology. Retrieved from <https://jolly2.s3.amazonaws.com/Research/Getting%20Ready%20for%20Reading.pdf>.
C, Crane, Et, Al. (1999). Improving early language and literacy skills: differential effects of an oral language versus a phonology with reading intervention. University of York. Retrieved from <https://jolly2.s3.amazonaws.com/Research/BowyerCrane%20etal2007proof.pdf>.
L, Farokhbakht. The Effect of Using Multisensory-based Phonics in Teaching Literacy on EFL Young Female/Male Learners' Early Reading Motivation. University of Isfahan. Retrieved from <https://jolly2.s3.amazonaws.com/Research/The%20Effect%20of%20Using%20Multisensory-based%20Phonics%20in%20Teaching%20Literacy%20on%20EFL%20Young%20FemaleMale%20Learners'%20Early%20Reading%20Motivation.pdf>.
N, Katechaiyo, et al. EFFECTS OF JOLLY PHONICS INSTRUCTION FOR PUPIL BOOK 1 ON REDING ABILITY OF THAI EFL YOUNG LEARNERS. Retrieved from <https://jolly2.s3.amazonaws.com/Research/Revealing%20the%20secrets%20of%20remarkable%20improvement%20of%20Thai%20EFL%20young%20learners_Aug.2021.pdf>.
Republic of Gambia. (2009). IMPACT ASSESSMENT OF INTERVENTIONS ON EARLY GRADE READING ABILITY (EGRA) IN SCHOOLS. Retrieved from <https://www.jollylearning.co.uk/evidence/research/>.
Government of Nigeria. (2014). REPORT ON THE MONITORING EXERCISE FOR THE IMPLEMENTATION OF JOLLY PHONICS APPROACH IN THE FEDERAL CAPITAL TERRITORY, ABUJA. Retrieved from <https://s3.amazonaws.com/jolly2/Research/Jolly+Phonics+in+FCT.pdf>.
N, Katechaiyo, Et al. EFFECTS OF JOLLY PHONICS INSTRUCTION FOR PUPIL BOOK 1 ON READING ABILITY OF THAI EFL YOUNG LEARNERS. Retrieved from <https://jolly2.s3.amazonaws.com/Research/Revealing%20the%20secrets%20of%20remarkable%20improvement%20of%20Thai%20EFL%20young%20learners_Aug.2021.pdf>.
-NRP. (2001). Teaching Children to Read: An Evidence Based Assessment of the Scientific Literature on Reading Instruction. United States Government. Retrieved from <https://www.nichd.nih.gov/sites/default/files/publications/pubs/nrp/Documents/report.pdf>.
CKLA:
Amplify. (2019). Amplify CKLA AZ grade 5 efficacy research report. Retrieved from <https://amplify.com/wp-content/uploads/2019/12/CKLA_AZ-grade-5-efficacy-research-report.pdf>.
CKLA. (2019). CK Early Literacy Pilot. Retrieved from <https://amplify.com/wp-content/uploads/2019/12/CKLA-Early-Literacy-Pilot.pdf>.
Cabell, S.Q., White, T.G., Kim, J., Hwang, H., & Gale, C. (2019, December). Impact of the Core Knowledge Language Arts read-aloud program on kindergarteners’ vocabulary, listening comprehension, and general knowledge. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Literacy Research Association, Tampa, FL.
Cabell, S. Q., & Hwang, H. (2020). Building Content Knowledge to Boost Comprehension in the Primary Grades. Reading Research Quarterly, 55, S99–S107. https://doi-org.ezproxy.lakeheadu.ca/10.1002/rrq.338
Elleman, A.M., Lindo, E.J., Morphy, P., & Compton, D.L. (2009). The impact of vocabulary instruction on passage-level comprehension of school-age children: A meta-analysis. Journal of Research on Educational Effectiveness, 2(1), 1–44. https://doi.org/10.1080/1934574080 2539200
S, Cabell. (Date not specified).Impact of a content-rich English language arts program on kindergarten students' language and knowledge. Florida State University. Retrieved from <https://www.triplesr.org/impact-content-rich-english-language-arts-program-kindergarten-students-language-and-knowledge>.
J, Wedman. (2004). Core Knowledge Curriculum and School Performance. University of Missour. Retrieved from <https://www.coreknowledge.org/wp-content/uploads/2016/12/CK_National_Study_2004.pdf>.
M, Abele. (2000). CORE KNOWLEDGE CURRICULUMFive-Year Analysis of Implementation and Effects in Five Maryland Schools. John Hopkins University.
Retrieved from <https://www.coreknowledge.org/wp-content/uploads/2016/12/FiveYearEffects_Maryland_2000.pdf>.
Wilson:
Wilson. (2022) Fundation. Retrieved from <https://www.wilsonlanguage.com/programs/fundations/>.
Fritts, J. L. (2016). Direct instruction and Orton-Gillingham reading methodologies:
Effectiveness of increasing reading achievement of elementary school students with learning
disabilities (Publication No. 10168236) [Master’s thesis, Northeastern University]. ProQuest Dissertations and Theses Global. Reuter,
H. B. (2006). Phonological awareness instruction for middle school students with disabilities:
A scripted multisensory intervention (Publication No. 3251867) [Master’s thesis,University of Oregon]. ProQuest Dissertations and Theses Global.
Torgesen, J., Schirm, A., Castner, L., Vartivarian, S., Mansfield, W., Myers, D., Stancavage,
F., Durno, D., Javorsky, R., & Haan, C. (2007). National assessment of Title I: Final
report. Volume II. Closing the reading gap: Findings from a randomized trial of four reading
interventions for striving readers (NCEE 2008-4013). National Center for Education
Evaluation and Regional Assistance. https://ies.ed.gov/ncee/pdf/20084013.pdf
Wanzek, J., & Roberts, G. (2012). Reading interventions with varying instructional emphases
for fourth graders with reading difficulties. Learning Disability Quarterly, 35(2), 90–101.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0731948711434047
Reading SImplified:
Bratsch-Hines, M., Vernon-Feagans, L., Pedonti, S., & Varghese, C. (2020). Differential Effects of the Targeted Reading Intervention for Students With Low Phonological Awareness and/or Vocabulary. Learning Disability Quarterly, 43(4), 214–226. https://doi-org.ezproxy.lakeheadu.ca/10.1177/073194871985868
Aiken. (2020). Targeted Reading Intervention Teacher Certification: An Approach to Building and Sustaining Teacher Expertise in Rural Schools. Literacy Research and Instruction., 59(4), 346–369.
Amendum, S. J., Bratsch, H. M., & Vernon, F. L. (2018). Investigating the Efficacy of a Web‐Based Early Reading and Professional Development Intervention for Young English Learners. Reading Research Quarterly, 53(2), 155–174. https://doi-org.ezproxy.lakeheadu.ca/10.1002/rrq.188
Vernon-Feagans, L., Gallagher, K., Ginsberg, M. C., Amendum, S., Kainz, K., Rose, J., & Burchinal, M. (2010). A Diagnostic Teaching Intervention for Classroom Teachers: Helping Struggling Readers in Early Elementary School. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice (Wiley-Blackwell), 25(4), 183–193. https://doi-org.ezproxy.lakeheadu.ca/10.1111/j.1540-5826.2010.00316.x
Vernon-Feagans, L., Kainz, K., Amendum, S., Ginsberg, M., Wood, T., & Bock, A. (2012). Targeted Reading Intervention: A Coaching Model to Help Classroom Teachers With Struggling Readers. Learning Disability Quarterly, 35(2), 102–114. https://doi.org/10.1177/0731948711434048
Amendum, S. J., Vernon-Feagans, L., & Ginsberg, M. C. (2011). The Effectiveness of a Technologically Facilitated Classroom-Based Early Reading Intervention. Elementary School Journal, 112(1), 107–131.
Vernon-Feagans, L., Kainz, K., Amendum, S., Ginsberg, M., Wood, T., & Bock, A. (2012). Targeted Reading Intervention: A Coaching Model to Help Classroom Teachers With Struggling Readers. Learning Disability Quarterly, 35(2), 102–114. https://doi-org.ezproxy.lakeheadu.ca/10.1177/0731948711434048
Vernon-Feagans, L., Kainz, K., Ginsberg, M., Hedrick, A., & Amendum, S. (2013). Live Webcam Coaching to Help Early Elementary Classroom Teachers Provide Effective Literacy Instruction for Struggling Readers: The Targeted Reading Intervention. Journal of Educational Psychology, 105(4), 1175–1187. https://doi-org.ezproxy.lakeheadu.ca/10.1037/a0032143
HMH
The Learning Company. (2020). HMH Into Reading Implementation Study Research Study Results 2019-2020. Retrieved from <https://s3.amazonaws.com/prod-hmhco-vmg-craftcms-public/research/Research-Results-Paper-Into-Reading-2019-2020-SY.pdf>.
R, Eddy, Et al. (2020). QUASI-EXPERIMENTAL DESIGN. Cobblestone. Retrieved from <https://s3.amazonaws.com/prod-hmhco-vmg-craftcms-public/research/HMH-Into-Reading-ESSA-Tier-2-QED-Study-Report.pdf>.
Filderman, M. J., Austin, C. R., Boucher, A. N., O’Donnell, K., & Swanson, E. A. (2022). A Meta-Analysis of the Effects of Reading Comprehension Interventions on the Reading Comprehension Outcomes of Struggling Readers in Third Through 12th Grades. Exceptional Children, 88(2), 163–184. https://doi.org/10.1177/00144029211050860
Rewards:
C, Buttler. (2013). The impact of REWARDS on reading skills of students with learning disabilities. California State University. <https://digitalcommons.csumb.edu/cgi/viewcontent.cgi?article=1402&context=caps_thes>.
M, Shippen. (2004). A Comparison of Two Direct Instruction Reading Programs for Urban Middle School Students. Remedial and Special Education. Retrieved from <http://mathenrich.pbworks.com/w/file/fetch/52804182/AComparison.pdf>.
Anita Archer, Mary Gleason, and Vicky Vachon. (No known date). Unidentified RCT Study. Broken link: <https://fcrr.org/FCRRReports/PDF/RewardsPlus.pdf>.
I, Klee. (2015). The Effect Using the REWARDS® Reading Program on Vowel Sounds, Word Part, and Prefix and Suffix Identification in Multi-Syllabic Words: A Case Report. Education Research Quarterly. Retrieved from <https://eric.ed.gov/?id=EJ1061951>.
R, Autzen. (2021). REWARDS: A Reading Intervention Program to Address Grade Level Reading Ability. Undergraduate Scholarly Showcase Proceedings. Retrieved from <https://journals.uc.edu/index.php/Undergradshowcase/article/view/4582/3456>.
R, Kerl. (2018). Timing is Everything: Adapting Rewards Intermediate For 4th And 5th Grade Striving Readers. Hamline University. Retrieved from <https://digitalcommons.hamline.edu/cgi/viewcontent.cgi?article=5436&context=hse_all>.
The Learning Company. (2020). HMH Into Reading Implementation Study Research Study Results 2019-2020. Retrieved from <https://s3.amazonaws.com/prod-hmhco-vmg-craftcms-public/research/Research-Results-Paper-Into-Reading-2019-2020-SY.pdf>.
R, Eddy, Et al. (2020). QUASI-EXPERIMENTAL DESIGN. Cobblestone. Retrieved from <https://s3.amazonaws.com/prod-hmhco-vmg-craftcms-public/research/HMH-Into-Reading-ESSA-Tier-2-QED-Study-Report.pdf>.
Filderman, M. J., Austin, C. R., Boucher, A. N., O’Donnell, K., & Swanson, E. A. (2022). A Meta-Analysis of the Effects of Reading Comprehension Interventions on the Reading Comprehension Outcomes of Struggling Readers in Third Through 12th Grades. Exceptional Children, 88(2), 163–184. https://doi.org/10.1177/00144029211050860
SPIRE:
M, Gallagher. (2019). S.P.I.R.E. Intensive Reading Intervention: A Comparative Analysis at Second Through Sixth. Spire. Retrieved from <https://eps.schoolspecialty.com/EPS/media/Site-Resources/Downloads/research-papers/Study-Intensive-Reading-Intervention-Gallagher-2019-SPIRE.pdf>.
M, Wilger. (2008). Spring Independent SchoolDistrict (ISD),TX 2007-2008 SchoolYear. Auto Skill International. Retrieved from <https://eps.schoolspecialty.com/EPS/media/Site-Resources/Downloads/studies/ES_Spring_TX.pdf>.
A, Grippi. (2006). Teaching the Disruptive Child to Read: An Evaluation of the SPIRE Reading Program. International Journal of Arts and Sciences. Retrieved from <https://eps.schoolspecialty.com/EPS/media/Site-Resources/Downloads/Miscellaneous/spire/teaching-the-disruptive-child.pdf?ext=.pdf>.
Words Their Way:
Eddy, R. M., Ruitman, T., Hankel, N., Matelski, M. H. & Schmalstig, M. (2011). Pearson Words Their Way: Word Study in Action Intervention Efficacy Study Final Report.
ELL Toolbox. (2022). Words Their Way. Retrieved from <http://www.elltoolbox.com/words-their-way.html#.YhW4XujMLIV>.
Take Flight:
Ring, J., Avrit, K., Black, J., Ring, J. J., Avrit, K. J., & Black, J. L. (2017). Take Flight: the evolution of an Orton Gillingham-based curriculum. Annals of Dyslexia, 67(3), 383–400. https://doi-org.ezproxy.lakeheadu.ca/10.1007/s11881-017-0151-9
Wonders:
McGraw Hill. (2016). Champaign Community Schools scores on NWEA MAP® reading assessment increased significantly from fall 2015 to spring 2016. Retrieved from <https://s3.amazonaws.com/ecommerce-prod.mheducation.com/unitas/school/explore/sites/wonders/efficacy-and-success-brochure.pdf>.
McGraw Hill. (2016). California ELA Test Score Analysis: Wonders Research Report. Retrieved from <https://s3.amazonaws.com/ecommerce-prod.mheducation.com/unitas/school/explore/sites/wonders/efficacy-and-success-brochure.pdf>.
Lexia:
Rachel Schechter, Paul Macaruso, Elizabeth R. Kazakoff & Elizabeth Brooke (2015) Exploration of a Blended Learning Approach to Reading Instruction for Low SES Students in Early Elementary Grades, Computers in the Schools, 32:3-4, 183-200, DOI: 10.1080/07380569.2015.1100652
Macaruso, P., Hook, P. E., & McCabe, R. (2006). The efficacy of computer-based supplementary phonics programs for advancing reading skills in at-risk elementary students. Journal of Research in Reading, 29(2), 162–172. https://doi-org.ezproxy.lakeheadu.ca/10.1111/j.1467-9817.2006.00282.x
Macaruso, P., & Rodman, A. (2011). Efficacy of Computer-Assisted Instruction for the Development of Early Literacy Skills in Young Children. Reading Psychology, 32(2), 172–196. https://doi-org.ezproxy.lakeheadu.ca/10.1080/02702711003608071
Hurwitz, L.B. (2020). Supporting Struggling and Non-Proficient Middle School Readers with the Lexia PowerUp Literacy Program. Concord, MA: Lexia Learning Systems LLC, A Rosetta Stone Company
S, Wilkes, Et al. (2016). Exploration of a Blended Lerning Approach to Reading Instruction in Second Grade. Edmedia. Retrieved from <https://www.lexialearning.com/user_area/content_media/raw/EdMediaPresentation_TitleI.pdf>.
Macaruso, P., & Walker, A. (2008). The Efficacy of Computer-Assisted Instruction for Advancing Literacy Skills in Kindergarten Children. Reading Psychology, 29(3), 266–287. https://doi-org.ezproxy.lakeheadu.ca/10.1080/02702710801982019
Schechter, R., Macaruso, P., Kazakoff, E.R. and Brooke, E. (2015). Exploration of a blended learning approach to reading instruction for low SES students in early elementary grades. Computers in the Schools, 32, 183–200.
Macaruso, P., & Rodman, A. (2011). Benefits of Computer-Assisted Instruction to Support Reading Acquisition in English Language Learners. Bilingual Research Journal, 34(3), 301–315. https://doi-org.ezproxy.lakeheadu.ca/10.1080/15235882.2011.622829
Paul Macaruso & Alyson Rodman (2009) Benefits of computer‐assisted instruction for struggling readers in middle school, European Journal of Special Needs Education, 24:1, 103-113, DOI: 10.1080/08856250802596774
Feng, L., Lindner, A., Ji, X. R., & Malatesha Joshi, R. (2019). The roles of handwriting and keyboarding in writing: a meta-analytic review. Reading & Writing, 32(1), 33–63. https://doi-org.ezproxy.lakeheadu.ca/10.1007/s11145-017-9749-x
Ehri, Linnea & Nunes, Simone & Willows, Dale & Schuster, Barbara & Yaghoub-Zadeh, Zohreh & Shanahan, Timothy. (2001). Phonemic Awareness Instruction Helps Children Learn to Read: Evidence From the National Reading Panel's Meta-Analysis. Reading Research Quarterly. 36. 250-287. 10.1598/RRQ.36.3.2.
Elleman, A.M., Lindo, E.J., Morphy, P., & Compton, D.L. (2009). The impact of vocabulary instruction on passage-level comprehension of school-age children: A meta-analysis. Journal of Research on Educational Effectiveness, 2(1), 1–44. https://doi.org/10.1080/1934574080 2539200
HMH. (2022). Research Into Reading Research Foundations. Retrieved from <https://www.hmhco.com/research/hmh-into-reading-research-foundations>.
-NRP. (2001). Teaching Children to Read: An Evidence Based Assessment of the Scientific Literature on Reading Instruction. United States Government. Retrieved from <https://www.nichd.nih.gov/sites/default/files/publications/pubs/nrp/Documents/report.pdf>.
Voyageur Sopris. (2022). Reading Intervention. Retrieved from <https://www.voyagersopris.com/literacy/rewards/overview>.
Open Court:
Skindrud, K., & Gersten, R. (2006). An Evaluation of Two Contrasting Approaches for Improving Reading Achievement in a Large Urban District. Elementary School Journal, 106(5), 389–407. https://doi-org.ezproxy.lakeheadu.ca/10.1086/505437
Stockard, J. (2010). Promoting Reading Achievement and Countering the “Fourth-Grade Slump”: The Impact of Direct Instruction on Reading Achievement in Fifth Grade. Journal of Education for Students Placed at Risk, 15(3), 218–240. https://doi.org/10.1080/10824669.2010.495687
Vaden-Kiernan, M., Borman, G., Caverly, S., Bell, N., Sullivan, K., Ruiz de Castilla, V., Fleming, G., Rodriguez, D., Henry, C., Long, T., & Hughes Jones, D. (2018). Findings From a Multiyear Scale-Up Effectiveness Trial of Open Court Reading. Journal of Research on Educational Effectiveness, 11(1), 109–132. https://doi-org.ezproxy.lakeheadu.ca/10.1080/19345747.2017.1342886
Borman, G. D., Dowling, N. M., & Schneck, C. (2008). A Multisite Cluster Randomized Field Trial of Open Court Reading. Educational Evaluation and Policy Analysis, 30(4), 389–407. https://doi.org/10.3102/0162373708326283
Remediation Plus:
Corcoran, R., & Ross, S.M. (2015). An Evaluation of the Effectiveness of the Remediation Plus Program on Improving Reading Achievement of Students in the Marinette (WI) School District.
Spelling Mastery:
Darch, C., Eaves, R. C., Crowe, D. A., Simmons, K., & Conniff, A. (2006). Teaching spelling to students with learning disabilities: A comparison of rule-based strategies versus traditional instruction. Journal of Direct Instruction, 6(1), 1–16.
Darch, C., & Simpson, R. G. (1990). Effectiveness of visual imagery versus rule-based strategies in teaching spelling to learning disabled students. Research in Rural Education, 7(1), 61–70.
Reading Mastery:
C, Schieffer. (2002). An Analysis of the Reading Mastery Program: Effective Components and Research Review. Journal of Direct Instruction. Volume 2: Issue 2. Retrieved from <https://www.nifdi.org/research/journal-of-di/volume-2-no-2-summer-2002/442-an-analysis-of-the-reading-mastery-program-effective-components-and-research-review/file.html>.
Yu, L., & Rachor, R. (2000, April). The two-year evaluation of the three-year Direct Instruction program, in an urban public school system. Presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association, New Orleans, LA.
O'Connor, R. E., Jenkins, J. R., Cole, K. N., & Mills, P. E. (1993). Two approaches to reading instruction with children with disabilities: does program design make a difference?. Exceptional children, 59(4), 312–323. https://doi.org/10.1177/001440299305900404
The Development of Early Academic Success: The Impact of Direct Instruction’s Reading Mastery. (2010). Journal of Behavior Assessment & Intervention in Children, 1(1), 2–24. https://doi-org.ezproxy.lakeheadu.ca/10.1037/h0100357
Corrective Reading:
Benner, G. J., Michael, E., Ralston, N. C., & Lee, E. O. (2022). The impact of supplemental word recognition strategies on students with reading difficulties. International Journal of Instruction, 15(1) 837-856. https://doi.org/10.29333/iji.2022.15148a.
Benner, Gregory & Kinder, Diane & Beaudoin, Kathleen & Stein, Marcy. (2005). The Effects of the "Corrective Reading Decoding" Program on the Basic Reading Skills and Social Adjustment of Students with High-Incidence Disabilities. Journal of Direct Instruction.
Lloyd, J., Cullinan, D., Heins, E. D., & Epstein, M. H. (1980). Direct Instruction: Effects on Oral and Written Language Comprehension. Learning Disability Quarterly, 3(4), 70–76. https://doi.org/10.2307/1510677